Skip to main content

Executive function skills predict motor competence in preschool children

Abstract

Background

Motor competence and executive function skills develop actively at preschool age. Both are important for socialization, school achievements, and well-being. However, the association between motor competence and executive functions has not yet been fully investigated in preschool children. This study aimed to explore which executive function skills may be predictors of motor competence and its components.

Methods

Two hundred seventy-two typically developing 5- to 6-year-old children (46% girls) participated in this study. The motor competence assessment tool 'Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2' was used on the Russian sample for the first time.

Results

The study revealed that higher motor inhibition, working memory, and age were significant predictors of higher motor competence, manual dexterity, and balance skills. Higher aiming and catching skills were predicted only by higher motor inhibition and age. The study also revealed that girls had higher manual dexterity and balance skills than boys. And boys had higher than girls aiming and catching skills.

Conclusion

The obtained result with the largest effect size allows us to assume that training in motor inhibition could probably help improve movement difficulties, especially in manual dexterity skills.

Clinical trial number

Not applicable.

Peer Review reports

Background

Preschool age is a period of active development of motor competence (MC) [1, 2]. MC describes proficiency in fundamental movement skills [3, 4]. MC plays a key role in human development [5]. MC contributes to children’s language, cognitive, and social development [6,7,8,9,10,11]. High MC in childhood promotes engagement in physical activity and gives more chances for successful participation in a healthy lifestyle and sports throughout the lifespan [12]. MC is necessary for children to explore and interact with their environment [13]. MC is important for day-to-day play [14]. Preschoolers with MC difficulties more often spend time as onlookers during play compared to children without such difficulties [15]. Thus, low MC can interfere with exploring the environment and culture. Nowadays, the formation of high MC in preschoolers is threatened due to a sedentary lifestyle, which is associated with excessive screen time [16]. Not only physical activity affects MC, but also other not-so-well-explored predictors. One may be executive function skills because they are the foundation for a child's development [17]. Understanding MC predictors is important to develop evidence-based interventions to improve motor development in children, which is critical for quality of life.

MC in preschool children is assessed based on the success of mastering fundamental movement skills according to age norms [18]. Fundamental movement skills are goal-directed organized movement patterns [19, 20]. Normal development of fundamental movement skills allows one to handle motor tasks optimally, focusing on the result of the movement. Fundamental movement skills include locomotor skills (movements to transport the body from one point to another, e.g., running, leaping, galloping, etc.), manipulative or object control skills (movements to give an object force or get force from an object, e.g., aiming, catching, kicking, etc.), and balance skills (static balance – ability to maintain a desired still positions, e.g. when standing on one leg; dynamic balance – ability to control the body as it moves in space, e.g. when climbing or walking along the curb) [21]. At senior preschool age, MC already includes not only gross motor fundamental motor skills but also manual dexterity skills (“the ability to make coordinated hand and finger movements to grasp and manipulate objects”) [22, 23]. According to Piaget and many other researchers, manual dexterity skills for preschool children are means of learning about the world is [13]. Fundamental motor skills as well as simple manual dexterity skills begin to establish at ages 2 to 7 [1, 23]. These skills (and therefore MC) are primarily acquired through play and imitation [21, 24]. Fundamental motor skills are the foundation for more complicated motor skills needed for play, leisure activities, physical activity, and sport [1].

MC in preschool children is affected by various factors [1]. A child's MC depends on individual genetic and biological factors, anthropometric characteristics, physical activity, physical fitness [12, 25,26,27,28]. The preschool environment (time in open spaces, structured activity experiences, and availability of outdoor playground space) is important for MC formation [29]. There is evidence that place of residence also affects MC: children living in the village tend to have higher MC as children living in the suburbs and downtown [30]. Children in low-income families, countries, and regions may disadvantage in MC development because of poorer health and other reasons [31]. There may be also culture differences in different countries affecting MC development, however, this topic has hardly been studied [31]. Age affects MC due to biological maturation [9, 32]. Another factor that potentially affects MC is sex. According to the most of the studies, preschool girls have higher proficiency in balance skills [1, 33,34,35,36,37] and manual dexterity skills [33,34,35,36,37] than preschool boys. Boys at 5–6 years are more proficient than girls in object control skills [34, 38, 39]. Regarding locomotor skills, several studies found that boys have better locomotor skills than girls [39], whereas another found the opposite [38]. However, several studies have found no sex differences in locomotor skills proficiency [40, 41], object control skills [42], balance skills [43]. Therefore, no conclusion has been reached on sex differences in preschoolers’ MC [38]. Studies have shown that MC is also interrelated with executive function skills [9, 44,45,46,47,48,49,50].

Preschool age is a period of active and rapid development of MC and executive function skills (EF skills) [51, 52]. EF skills – “a set of general-purpose control processes that regulate one’s thoughts and behaviors” [53]. The conceptualization of EF skills is a polemical topic. Some researchers divide EF skills into cold (cognitive) and hot (affective) [54, 55]. Baggetta and Alexander [56] proposed a distinction between domain-general and domain-specific EF skills. There are unitary and modular views of EF skills [52]. The unitary view postulates that there are high intercorrelations among various EF skills. Moreover, some factor analytic studies showed that EF skills are a single cognitive ability [52, 57]. On the contrary, the modular view postulates that EF skills are independent modules. In sum, there are different EF skills models in terms of EF skills number, nature, and degree of separation or unity [52, 58].

Most researchers concur that EF skills encompass various mental processes: inhibition, working memory, cognitive flexibility, attention, planning, self-monitoring, self-regulation, and initiation [53, 59]. The most known and cited EF skills model is the model by Miyake et al. [60] (unity-but-diversity model). Initially, this model was proposed for adults, but it has been proven to apply to preschool children [51, 61]. According to the model by Miyake et al. [60], there are three core EF skills: inhibition (the ability to suppress distractions that aren't relevant to the objective; inhibition is divided into cognitive and motor), working memory (the ability to hold and process information required for current cognitive tasks; working memory is divided into verbal and visual), and cognitive flexibility (the ability to shift and switch thoughts, behaviors, and emotions in response to environmental changes). Mental processes such as planning, reasoning, and problem-solving are built from these core EF skills [51, 52]. According to the unity-but-diversity model, EF skills are connected by hierarchical relationships. Working memory is the first in the hierarchy, followed by inhibition. Cognitive flexibility develops later, in the late preschool period [52]. The Miyake et al. [60] EF skills model was used as the theoretical framework of the current research.

EF skills development at preschool age predicts academic success, life achievement, health, and well-being in adulthood [62,63,64,65,66,67,68]. EF skills in the preschool period play a crucial role in ensuring development in general [17] and motor development in particular [69].

A small number of research have examined the association between EF skills and MC in preschoolers. Escolano-Pérez et al. [1] found in a study with 43 5–6-years-old Spanish children that children with higher working memory obtained higher scores on locomotor skills, and children with higher inhibition scores – had higher balance scores. Object control skills were not affected by the level of EF skills. This study measured only these two EF skills with a tool in which the EF skills of children are teacher-rated. In the research by Han et al. [2] with 394 children aged 3–5 years from Shanxi (China), fundamental motor skills were studied as EF skills predictors. It was shown that locomotor skills are significant predictors of working memory, inhibition, and cognitive flexibility, while object control skills are only significant predictors of inhibition. In a study by Oberer et al. [46], positive correlations were found between MC (gross motor skills and manual dexterity skills), and all evaluated EF skills (inhibition, switching, updating) in 156 6-year-old children from Switzerland. Cook et al. [48] found that inhibition was associated with locomotor and object control skills, whereas working memory was only associated with locomotor skills. There was no association between motor skills and cognitive flexibility. Only locomotor and object control skills were evaluated in this study to assess MC. One hundred twenty-nine children aged 3–6 years from urban and rural low‐income settings in South Africa participated in this study. A cross-sectional study of the association between gross motor skills and EF skills in 25 normal-weight and 24 overweight 3–5-years-old children from Brazil was conducted by Fernandes et al. [70]. In overweight preschoolers, worse cognitive flexibility, working memory, planning, and problem-solving were associated with worse gross motor skills. In normal-weight children, there are no associations between EF skills and gross motor skills. Thus, most studies have shown an association between inhibition and MC, an association between working memory and MC is less common, and an association between cognitive flexibility and MC is found in isolated studies.

There are different explanations for the association between MC and EF skills. The first theoretical framework that explains how MC and EF skills are interrelated is reciprocity [71]. Reciprocity refers to the co-development of EF skills and MC through interaction with the environment [71, 72]. Learning motor skills support EF skills development, which supports motor development [71]. The second theoretical framework is automaticity [71]. Automaticity refers to the idea that complex skills mastering depends on more rudimentary skills [71]. It means that motor and cognitive processes compete for limited attentional resources when both motor and cognitive components are required [71, 72]. Automaticity of motor skills frees up attentional resources to perform cognitive tasks and vice versa. The third framework on the interrelation of MC and EF skills is from neuroscience. MC and EF skills have a shared underlying neuronal network: dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, cerebellum, and connecting structures [71, 73]. To sum up, the causal mechanisms of the association between MC and EF skills are bi-directional. However, the effects must still be fully understood [74].

Taking into account all the above, the association between MC components and EF skills has yet to be thoroughly studied in preschoolers [1, 5, 72]. Previous studies of this relationship have been conducted mainly on schoolchildren. The extent to which the results can be generalized to preschoolers is still unclear [9]. Furthermore, studies that involved preschoolers usually only included some EF skills and components of MC [9]. Studies usually selectively focused on one or two EF skills and separately locomotor, object control, balance, or manual dexterity skills [9]. The present study examined which EF skills may predict MC and its components in typically developing preschool children. The research questions were: (1) Are there any sex differences in MC? (2) Which EF skills are predictors of MC? (3) Which EF skills are predictors of each MC component?

Methods

Participants

Three hundred fourteen children without developmental disabilities were recruited to participate in the MC assessment. Still, three of them did not complete all the tasks because their caregivers took them home. Of the 311 children who passed the MC assessment, 272 participated in the EF skills assessment. Of the 311 children who passed the MC assessment, 39 were absent from kindergarten on the days of the EF skills assessment due to illness or other reasons. Therefore, the sample consisted of 272 typically developing preschool children (46% girls) aged 5 to 6 years. All children attended public kindergartens in Moscow. A questionnaire for caregivers showed that most of the children were from middle SES families: 98% of mothers had higher education, 77% of families had a medium income level. However, it is important to consider that family income in this study was estimated subjectively from parents’ words. A comprehensive overview of the demographic variables is in Table 1.

Table 1 Demographic variables

Measurements

Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2 (M-ABC-2) [23] was used to assess MC. Age band 1 (from 3 years, 0 months to 6 years, 11 months) comprises eight tasks belonging to three components: three tasks checking manual dexterity skills, two tasks checking aiming and catching skills, and three tasks checking balance skills. Before each task, children receive verbal explanation and demonstration of the task performance. Moreover, after explanation and demonstration, children make a training trial until they understand the instructions clearly. In the first task checking manual dexterity skills, the child should put 12 plastic coins in the bank box as quickly as possible. In the second task, the child should lift the lace and thread 12 beads as quickly as possible. In both tasks, the result in seconds is recorded. In the third task, the child should draw a single continuous line from point A to point B, following the trail without crossing the boundaries. The number of errors is recorded. In the first task checking aiming and catching skills, the child should catch a 200-g beanbag thrown by the examiner with two hands. The examiner should stand on the special mat separated by 1.8 m from the child's mat. There are 10 hit attempts, and the number of successful attempts out of 10 is recorded. In the second task, the child should throw the beanbag 10 times, attempting to land it on any part of the target mat. Also, the number of successful attempts out of 10 is recorded. In the first task checking balance skills, the child should stand on one leg on the mat with freely held arms for up to 30 s. The standing foot must be fixed, and the free foot must be off the floor. The number of seconds, up to 30, the child maintains balance is recorded. In the second task, the child should walk with heels raised along the 4.5 m line on the floor without stepping off the line. The number of correct consecutive steps from the beginning of the line is recorded. In the last task, the child should make five consecutive continuous jumps from mat to mat, taking off and landing with the feet together each time. The number of correct consecutive jumps is recorded.

Regarding the test scoring, the row scores are converted to the standard scores for each of the 8 tasks. This conversion differs for children aged 5 years 0 months – 5 years 11 months and children aged 6 years 0 months – 6 years 11 months. The standard scores for three test components and MC components are determined: manual dexterity skills – maximum standard score 57; aiming and catching skills – maximum standard score 38; balance skills – maximum standard score 57; MC—maximum standard score 152.

The NEPSY-II 'Sentences Repetition' subtest [75] assessed verbal working memory. The child must remember one by one 17 sentences that progressively become more difficult, grow in length, and become more grammatically intricate. 2 points are given for each correctly repeated sentence, 1 point for a sentence with not more than 2 errors, and 0 points for more errors (maximum total score = 34).

The NEPSY-II subtest 'Memory for Designs' was used [75] to assess visual working memory. There are four trials (with 4, 6, 6, and 8 images) where the child must select the appropriate cards and place them on a grid in the same location as previously shown (maximum total score = 120).

The NEPSY-II subtest 'Inhibition' [75] assessed cognitive inhibition. The child must name forty figures (squares, circles, and arrows) that are the opposite of those actually pictured. Time devoted to each task and the number of errors (both corrected by a child and not corrected) are recorded. These parameters are converted into a combined scaled score (from 1 to 20 points) based on corresponding tables.

The NEPSY-II subtest 'Statue' [75] assessed motor inhibition. The child has to stay immobile for 75 s without being disturbed by a special sound stimulus (knocking, coughing, or the sound of a pen falling on the floor). The child gets 0 to 2 points for successfully following instructions for each 5-s period (maximum score = 30).

'Dimensional Change Card Sort' [76] assessed cognitive flexibility. There are 24 cards: 12 with rabbits and 12 with boats. On six cards, rabbits are blue, and on the other six cards, they are red (on a white background). The same goes for the cards with boats. Half of the cards have a black frame. The initial task is to separate 6 cards by color (put red cards on one side and blue ones on the other). The following task is to separate 6 cards by shape (boats and rabbits separately). The final task with 12 cards is to switch depending on the presence of the frame from sorting cards by color to sorting them by form. The child gains 1 point for each correctly sorted card (maximum total score = 24).

A questionnaire for caregivers was used to collect sociodemographic data about children and their family’s socioeconomic status (SES). There were two questions about SES. The first question, "What is your family income?” had the following answer options: "below average," "average," "above average," and "other." The second question, "What is the education level of the mother of the child?” had the following answer options: “Primary school," "High school," “Bachelor’s degree," "Master’s degree," "PhD," and "Other." Also, there were two questions about sociodemographic data: the first about the child's date of birth and the second about the child's sex. Data on the family’s SES were collected to describe the sample. Data on SES is important as children's EF skills are known to be related to it [77]. Data on a child's date of birth and sex were used in the analysis to examine sex differences and the role of age in MC.

The M-ABC-2 was used on a Russian sample for the first time, so UK norms were used [23]. All the NEPSY-II subtests used in the study and 'Dimensional Change Card Sort' tool have been translated and validated for the Russian population [78].

Procedure

The MC assessment was the first, and the EF skills assessment was conducted a week later. Both assessments were performed between 8 and 11 am. Specially trained examiners administered the assessment. All examiners successfully passed an exam, proving that they did all the assessment tasks correctly. Before the assessments, all caregivers provided written informed consent for their child’s participation in the study. After the assessment, the children's caregivers received a questionnaire about sociodemographic data via email.

The MC assessment was conducted in the sports hall and took about 25–40 min with one child. It was administered individually, but there were simultaneously three examiners and three children in the sports hall. The first examiner administered tasks to one child, checking manual dexterity skills. Then, the second examiner administered the same child tasks, checking aiming and catching skills. Finally, the third examiner tasks checking balance skills. After one child completed the tasks checking manual dexterity skills and moved on to the second examiner, a new child came to the first examiner, and so forth. To avoid distracting each other, the children did tasks with their backs to each other and in different areas of the sports hall. The assistant brought the children to the sports hall individually and then, after assessment, took each child away. All children wore their usual sports clothing and shoes during the tests.

The assessment of EF skills was carried out individually for each childy. Eight examiners were preliminarily trained to perform this EF skills assessment. The EF skills assessment was carried out using a specially created application for tablets [79]. The process of instructing the child and recording his or her answers for each task was done automatically using the application. The examiner only turned on the application and monitored how the child performed the tasks. In the application, the tasks were run to all the children in the same order. This automation ensured the unification of assessment by all eight examiners. Also, the unification was ensured because all the examiners used the same tablet model (Samsung Galaxy tab 6). The EF skills assessment was divided into two 20-min sessions with three days of break between. The first session included verbal working memory and cognitive inhibition tasks. The second session included visual working memory, cognitive flexibility, and motor inhibition tasks. Children were tested in a quiet room in their kindergarten (in the bedroom or psychologist’s office).

The Ethics Committee of the Faculty of Psychology at Lomonosov Moscow State approved this study and its consent procedures (approval No: 2023/18).

Data analysis strategy

Data analysis was carried out in the Jamovi 2.0.0.0 software. For all the tests the level of significance was set to p = 0.05. The normality of the distribution was examined with the Shapiro–Wilk test. Since the data were not normally distributed, nonparametric methods were used onwards. Descriptive statistics (Mean, Standard Deviation) and Spearman correlation were used for preliminary analysis to explore five EF tests measures, four MC indexes measures, and participants’ age, and their associations. Strengths of the Spearman correlation (rs) were interpreted as follows: rs = 0.1–0.39 weak correlation, rs = 0.4–0.69 moderate, and rs ≥ 0.7 strong [80]. A Mann–Whitney U test was performed to evaluate whether MC, EF skills, and age differed by sex. Sex differences in EF skills and age were examined to rule out the possibility that sex differences in MC were due to differences in EF skills and age. Rank biserial correlation (rb) was used to calculate the effect size for the Mann–Whitney U test. Interpretation of rank biserial correlation was as follows: rb < 0.10 represented a very small effect, rb = 0.10–0.29 small effect, rb = 0.30–0.49 moderate effect, and rb ≥ 0.5 large effect [81]. The general linear model was performed to test EF skills and participants’ age as MC predictors. Three general linear models with significant MC predictors from the previous general linear model were performed to test EF skills and age as manual dexterity, aiming, catching skills, and balance skills predictors. Partial eta squared (η2p) was used to calculate the effect size for the General Linear Model. Values of 0.01, 0.06, and 0.14 indicated small, medium, or large effects [82].

Results

Preliminary analysis: descriptive statistics and correlation analysis for EF skills and MC

Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics for the EF and MC skills measures. Shapiro–Wilk test indicated that all parameters except verbal working memory and aiming and catching skills were not distributed normally (see Table 2).

Table 2 Means, Standard Deviations and Shapiro–Wilk test for EF skills variables and MC variables

Correlations between the included variables are shown in Table 3. There were weak positive correlations between all EF skills (except cognitive inhibition and motor inhibition with no correlation between them). There were weak positive correlations between MC components, and all MC components were strongly correlated with MC (except aiming and catching skills with moderate correlation). There were weak positive correlations between manual dexterity skills and visual working memory, motor inhibition, and cognitive flexibility. The same is true regarding balance skills and motor competence. In the case of aiming and catching skills, there were weak correlations only with visual working memory and motor inhibition. The age in months was significantly weakly correlated with all the included variables (except verbal working memory).

Table 3 Spearman correlations among the EF skills variables, MC variables, and participants’ age

Sex differences in MC

Table 4 presents differences between boys and girls. Girls had a higher level of manual dexterity skills and balance skills than boys. Boys had a higher level of aiming and catching skills than girls. The effect size of these results was small. There were no sex differences in the EF skills and age.

Table 4 Sex differences in the EF skills variables, MC variables, and participants’ age

EF skills as predictors of MC

The general linear model based on the EF skills and age as MC predictors was significant, but the effect size was small (Table 5). As age was significantly correlated with MC, age was also added to the model as a potential MC predictor. The result of this general linear model demonstrated that visual working memory (small effect size), motor inhibition (medium effect size), and age (medium effect size) are significant MC predictors (Table 5).

Table 5 General linear model based on the EF skills, participants’ age, and sex as MC predictors

EF skills as predictors of each MC component

Significant MC predictors (visual working memory, motor inhibition, age) from the previous model were included in the four final models: a general linear model for MC and three separate models for each of the three MC components: manual dexterity skills, aiming and catching skills, and balance skills (Table 6).

Table 6 General linear models for MC and its components are based on main MC predictors

In the general linear model for MC, motor inhibition and age had a medium effect size as MC predictors and visual working memory had a small effect size (Table 6). In the general linear model for manual dexterity skills, motor inhibition had a medium effect size as a manual dexterity skills predictor. In contrast, visual working memory and age had a small effect size (Table 6). In the general linear model for aiming and catching skills, motor inhibition, and age were predictors with a small effect size, and visual working memory was not a predictor (Table 6). All three predictors (visual working memory, motor inhibition, age) had a small effect size in the general linear model for balance skills (Table 6).

Discussion

Sex differences in MC

The first research question of the present study was about sex differences in MC.

In the present study, sex differences in MC were found: girls had higher levels of manual dexterity and balance skills, and boys had higher levels of aiming and catching skills. No sex differences in age and EF skills were found. Due to this, it can be argued that the sex differences in MC were not due to the sex differences in age or EF skills. These sex differences in MC can be attributed to how preschoolers usually spend their leisure time: boys play more with balls, while girls do activities such as drawing, rope skipping, and other balancing games [35, 83]. The same sex differences were revealed in the majority of the studies [33,34,35,36,37, 84]. However, in the study by Singh et al. [43], no differences in balance skills between preschool boys and girls were found [43]. In the study, children (30 boys and 19 girls) were aged 3–4 years. The possible reason for the absence of differences between boys and girls in balance skills in this study is the different ages of children. It can be assumed that at 5–6 years of age, the sex differences in balance skills become more pronounced at 3–4 years. Also, there were studies showed no sex differences in object control skills [42]. In the study by Bakhtiar [42], there were no significant sex differences for object control skills, but boys were slightly higher than girls in these skills. In this study, children were one year older (67 children 6–7-years old) than in the present study. Object control skills were evaluated with the following tasks: striking, stationary dribbling, catching, kicking, overhand throw, and underhand roll [42]. In the present study, object control skills were evaluated only with aiming and catching tasks. Due to the different number and complexity of the tasks, it is not entirely appropriate to compare the results obtained in the present study and the study by Bakhtiar [42]. Hence, a broader picture of the sex differences in MC of preschoolers is needed. It is important to note that two of three MC components (manual dexterity and balance skills) evaluated in M-ABC-2 are components in which girls tend to be superior [35]. In studies with the M-ABC-2 tool, preschool girls are more likely to get a higher score in tasks checking manual dexterity and balance skills, and boys in tasks checking to aim and to catch. Clinicians who use M-ABC-2 to identify children at risk of motor impairment should be aware that boys and girls do not perform M-ABC-2 in a similar way.

EF skills as predictors of MC

The second research question was to determine which EF skills were MC predictors. This research revealed that MC was predicted by visual working memory, motor inhibition, and age. Motor inhibition as an MC predictor had the largest effect size. This outcome is consistent with the results and ideas of other researchers [5, 9, 47, 85]. This result is related to the fact that there is a congruence between MC and motor inhibition: both require inhibitory control and body movements [47]. In the M-ABC-2 test, the MC score is the sum of the scores for three components. Therefore, to discuss EF skills as MC predictors, it makes sense to consider each MC component in more detail.

EF skills as predictors of each MC component

The third research question was to determine which EF skills were predictors of each MC component: manual dexterity skills, aiming and catching skills, and balance skills.

EF skills as MC components predictors: EF skills as manual dexterity skills predictors

Manual dexterity skills were predicted by visual working memory, motor inhibition, and age. The effect size of motor inhibition as a manual dexterity skills predictor was the largest. The nature of the M-ABC-2 tasks checking manual dexterity skills might explain this. First, only these tasks were required to perform as quickly as possible. Unnecessary movements lengthen the time it takes to complete the task. This means that all movement features play a role in performing tasks checking manual dexterity skills. Second, in the tasks checking manual dexterity skills, the finest control of movement execution is required. It is in these tasks that the result and efficiency are more dependent on the accuracy of movement execution and movement trajectory. Some imprecision is acceptable in the tasks checking aiming and catching skills, and balance skills. In these tasks only achieving the movement goal matters. For example, when catching the beanbag or standing on one leg, the child can make free body movements if they do not disrupt the instruction or interfere with the outcome. However, in the tasks checking manual dexterity skills, hand movements must be very precise. Third, manual dexterity skills in preschool are not yet automated; therefore, they require more significant involvement in more EF skills, especially motor inhibition [2, 9, 72]. The finding of the study that visual working memory was a predictor of manual dexterity skills is consistent with previous studies [5, 9, 50, 85, 86]. The connection between visual working memory and manual dexterity skills can be explained by the tasks checking manual dexterity skills requiring an accurate visual perception of the stimuli and their location. In addition, successful task performance means that visual and motor information must be integrated [72]. For example, to quickly and accurately put coins into a slot, the child needs to keep the exact location of the coins and all their characteristics in working memory to adjust the movement of the hand to these parameters. It is the same as for other tasks checking manual dexterity skills.

EF skills as predictors of each MC component: EF skills as aiming and catching skills predictors

Aiming and catching skills were predicted by motor inhibition and age. Age had the largest effect size, but this effect was small. This result can be explained by normal maturation processes and similar developmental timelines for EF skills and motor development during preschool [9]. Visual working memory was a predictor for the other two MC components but not for aiming and catching skills. Visual working memory is less involved in the two M-ABC-2 tasks checking aiming and catching skills. It can be assumed that it is because, in these tasks, it is not necessary to keep small visual details and their mutual location in the visual working memory. Moreover, tasks checking aiming and catching skills are the most similar to the exercises that children do in sports classes with a ball. This means that these movements are more familiar and automated than the movements required for other M-ABC-2 tasks. Also, only in tasks checking aiming and catching skills does an examiner participate in movements with a child when they throw a beanbag to the child 10 times (catching task). In all other tasks, an examiner only watches the child's movements. It can be assumed that such an examiner's involvement implies that the child needs to use his working memory less than in the other tasks. If more object control skills were assessed (not only aiming and catching) in the study, the EF skills would play a more important role as predictors.

EF skills as predictors of each MC component: EF skills as balance skills predictors

Balance skills were predicted by visual working memory, motor inhibition, and age. Motor inhibition had the largest effect size, but all three predictors had a small effect. Other studies have also shown that motor inhibition [5, 9, 47, 85] and visual working memory were predictors of balance skills [5, 9, 50, 85, 86]. In one of the M-ABC-2 tasks checking balance skills, motor inhibition is especially required when standing on one leg for up to 30 s with the standing foot fixed and the free foot off the floor. This task was difficult for most of the children in the study sample. The connection between visual working memory and balance skills can be explained by the fact that one M-ABC-2 task checking balance skills (walking heels raised) requires an accurate visual perception of the line taped to the floor and the location of the feet. It is important to note that static and dynamic balance were not analyzed separately in the current study. In the M-ABC-2 tool, the general balance skills standard score (which is the sum of scores from the three tasks checking balance skills) was used. And the same goes for manual dexterity and balance skills.

EF skills which were not predictors of MC

The present study revealed that cognitive inhibition, verbal working memory, and cognitive flexibility were not predictors of MC and its components. Furthermore, no correlations existed between these EF skills, MC, and its components. The absence of these associations was shown in other studies [5, 47, 48, 85, 87, 88]. The examiner stays nearby while the child completes all the M-ABC-2 tasks, carefully observes the completion, and does some tasks with the child. Therefore, the child does not need to actively use cognitive inhibition when doing M-ABC-2 tasks. The absence of a significant association between verbal working memory and MC, as well as cognitive flexibility and MC, may be related to the M-ABC-2 assessment procedure: all the instructions are explained to the child verbally and through demonstration. The task begins only after the child understands the instructions clearly and after the training trial. Therefore, verbal working memory and cognitive flexibility are challenged. The connection between cognitive flexibility and MC was shown only in one reviewed study [9]. The study's authors supposed that their result could be explained by the nature of motor tasks in their research [9]. They used the Bruininks-Oseretsky Test of Motor Proficiency, second edition (BOT-2) [89]. The test tasks were less familiar to the children; therefore, they had to rapidly change their attention between simultaneous goals for successful task performance [9]. Here are tasks from BOT-2: touching nose with index fingers eyes closed; pivoting thumbs and index fingers; walking forward on a line; one-legged side hopping; push-ups, and fine motor tasks (coloring a star; drawing a line through a path; stringing blocks; copying overlapping circles; copying a diamond; catching a ball with one hand; dribbling a ball using alternate hands) [9]. Compared to M-ABC-2 used in the present study, these tasks are less like everyday play and exercise. It can be assumed that M-ABC-2 performance does not place high demands on cognitive flexibility. Additionally, it can be suggested that cognitive flexibility was not a predictor of MC when using the M-ABC-2 because cognitive flexibility was comparatively poorly developed compared to other EF skills in preschool age. Cognitive flexibility develops at an older age after working memory and inhibition [48, 52].

Limitations and strengths of the study

The present study has limitations. First, the current study did not show causality of the association between MC and EF skills, nor showed a possible reciprocal association between MC and EF skills. Thus, longitudinal research is required. Second, most of the effect sizes of the results in the study were small. Small effect sizes point to limited practical applications of the results. Third, there was no data about individual genetic, biological, and sociological elements that affect MC (e.g., level of physical activity, physical fitness, participation in sports or dance classes, etc.) [5]. Almost all the children were from middle or high-SES families. However, the results may differ in children from low-SES families [48]. Fourth, M-ABC-2 tool was not validated for the Russian population. So, standard scores equivalents of raw scores on each test item were calculated according to UK norms [23]. However, there might be cultural differences in motor performance [34, 48, 90], so standard scores equivalent to raw scores for the Russian sample are needed in the future. Fifth, there are no separate norms for boys and girls in M-ABC-2, while motor skills do not develop similarly in boys and girls at this age.

One of the strengths of the current study was that the tool 'Movement Assessment Battery for Children-2' was used on the Russian sample for the first time. Obtaining data on M-ABC-2 for children from different countries is scientifically valuable because there are shown to be cross-cultural differences in MC [30, 31, 34, 48, 90]. Furthermore, the number of boys and girls was nearly the same, therefore equally representing both sexes. Finally, the tools used to assess EF skills were validated for the Russian population and recognized as valid and reliable.

Conclusions

This cross-sectional design study examined whether EF skills predict MC in preschool children. Results indicated that higher visual working memory, motor inhibition, and age were predictors of higher MC. Visual working memory, motor inhibition, and age-predicted manual dexterity and balance skills. Aiming and catching skills were predicted by motor inhibition and age. The outcome was that motor inhibition was manual dexterity, and the MC predictor had the greatest practical value. Because this result had the largest effect size – medium close to large. This outcome could imply that training in motor inhibition could help improve deficits in MC, especially in manual dexterity skills. Future studies would benefit from longitudinal design. Likewise, assessing data about different factors that can affect MC would be prudent. Also, the authors suggest that future studies validate M-ABC-2 on Russian samples and develop separate norms for boys and girls.

Data availability

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

References

  1. Escolano-Pérez E, Sánchez-López CR, Herrero-Nivela ML. Teacher-Rated Executive Functions, Gender and Relative Age: Independent and Interactive Effects on Observed Fundamental Motor Skills in Kindergarteners. Front Psychol. 2022;13: 848525.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  2. Han X, Zhao M, Kong Z, Xie J. Association between fundamental motor skills and executive function in preschool children: A cross-sectional study. Front Psychol. 2022;13: 978994.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  3. Cattuzzo MT, Dos Santos HR, Re AH, de Oliveira IS, Melo BM, de Sousa MM, et al. Motor competence and health related physical fitness in youth: a systematic review. J Sci Med Sport. 2016;19(2):123–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Utesch T, Bardid F, Büsch D, Strauss B. The relationship between motor competence and physical fitness from early childhood to early adulthood: a meta-analysis. Sports Med. 2019;49(4):541–51.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Malambo C, Nová A, Clark C, Musálek M. Associations between fundamental movement skills, physical fitness, motor competency, physical activity, and executive functions in preschool age children: a systematic review. Children (Basel). 2022;9(7):1059.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Rosenbaum DA, Carlson RA, Gilmore RO. Acquisition of intellectual and perceptual-motor skills. Annu Rev Psychol. 2001;52(1):453–70.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Bar-Haim Y, Bart O. Motor function and social participation in kindergarten children. Soc Dev. 2006;15(2):296–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Iverson JM. Developing language in a developing body: the Relationship between motor development and language development. J Child Lang. 2010;37(2):229–61.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Gandotra A, Kotyuk E, Bizonics R, Khan I, Petánszki M, Cserjesi R, et al. An exploratory study of the Relationship between motor skills and indicators of cognitive and socio-emotional development in preschoolers. Eur J Dev Psychol. 2022;20(1):50–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Macdonald K, Milne N, Orr R, Pope R. Associations between motor proficiency and academic performance in mathematics and reading in year 1 school children: a cross-sectional study. BMC Pediatr. 2020;20:69.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  11. Williams KE, Howard SJ. Proximal and distal predictors of self-regulatory change in children aged 4 to 7 years. BMC Pediatr. 2020;20:226.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  12. Stodden DF, Goodway JD, Langendorfer SJ, Roberton MA, Rudisill ME, Garcia C, et al. A developmental perspective on the role of motor skill competence in physical activity: an emergent relationship. Quest. 2008;60(2):290–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Piaget J, Inhelder B. L’image Mentale chez L’enfant. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France; 1966.

    Google Scholar 

  14. Larkin D, Summers J. Implications of movement difficulties for social interaction, physical activity, play and sports. In: Dewey D, Tupper DE, editors. Developmental motor disorders: a neuropsychological perspective. New York: Guilford Press; 2004. p. 443–60.

    Google Scholar 

  15. Kennedy-Behr A, Rodger S, Mickan S. Physical and social play of preschool children with and without coordination difficulties: preliminary findings. Br J Occup Ther. 2011;74(7):348–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Webster EK, Martin CK, Staiano AE. Fundamental motor skills, screen-time, and physical activity in preschoolers. J Sport Health Sci. 2019;8(2):114–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Garon N, Bryson SE, Smith IM. Executive function in preschoolers: a review using an integrative framework. Psychol Bull. 2008;134(1):31–60.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Herrmann C, Heim C, Seelig H. Construct and correlates of basic motor competencies in primary school-aged children. J Sport Health Sci. 2019;8(1):63–70.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Burton AW, Miller DE. Movement skill assessment. Champaign: Human Kinetics; 1998.

    Google Scholar 

  20. Lawson C, Eyre ELJ, Tallis J, Duncan MJ. Fundamental movement skill proficiency among british primary school children: analysis at a behavioral component level. Percept Mot Skills. 2021;128(2):625–48.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  21. Lubans DR, Morgan PJ, Cliff DP, Barnett LM, Okely AD. Fundamental movement skills in children and adolescents: review of associated health benefits. Sports Med. 2010;40(12):1019–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Zhang A, Chen X, Zhao D, Zhang Y. The Association Between Motor Competence and Inhibitory Control in Preschool Children. Children. 2024;11:1537.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  23. Henderson SE, Sugden DA, Barnett AL. Movement assessment battery for children-2. In: Examiner’s manual. 2nd ed. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment; 2007.

  24. Staples KL, MacDonald M, Zimmer C. Assessment of motor behavior among children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Int Rev Res Dev Disa. 2012;42:179–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  25. Williams HG, Pfeiffer KA, O’Neill JR, Dowda M, McIver KL, Brown WH, et al. Motor skill performance and physical activity in preschool children. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2008;16(6):1421–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Schmutz EA, Leeger-Aschmann CS, Kakebeeke TH, Zysset AE, Messerli-Bürgy N, Stülb K, et al. Motor competence and physical activity in early childhood: stability and relationship. Front Public Health. 2020;8:39.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  27. Webster EK, Sur I, Stevens A, Robinson LE. Associations between body composition and fundamental motor skill competency in children. BMC Pediatr. 2021;21:444.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  28. Spring KE, Carroll AV, Wadsworth DD. The Relationship in early childhood body composition and physical activity levels regarding fundamental motor skill development. BMC Pediatr. 2023;23:461.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  29. True L, Pfeiffer KA, Dowda M, Williams HG, Brown WH, O’Neill JR, et al. Motor competence and characteristics within the preschool environment. J Sci Med Sport. 2017;20(8):751–5.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  30. Mohammadi M, Elahipanah F, Amani-shalamzari S. The role of the cultural environment in the development of physical literacy and physical activity of Iranian children. BMC Pediatr. 2023;23:477.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  31. Xia X, Chao L, Nan C, Yin X, Zheng H, Zheng Z. Fundamental motor skills of kindergarten children in different environments and ethnic groups in Northwest China. BMC Pediatr. 2022;22:423.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  32. Navarro-Patón R, Mecías-Calvo M, Rodríguez Fernández JE, Arufe-Giráldez V. Relative age effect on motor competence in children aged 4–5 years. Children (Basel). 2021;8(2):115.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Hirata S, Kita Y, Yasunaga M, Suzuki K, Okumura Y, Okuzumi H, et al. Applicability of the Movement Assessment Battery for Children-Second Edition (MABC-2) for Japanese Children Aged 3–6 Years: a Preliminary Investigation Emphasizing Internal Consistency and Factorial Validity. Front Psychol. 2018;9: 1452.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  34. Ke L, Du W, Wang Y, Duan W, Hua J, Barnett AL. The Movement ABC-2 Test in China: comparison with UK norms for 3–10 year olds. Res Dev Disabil. 2020;105: 103742.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Smits-Engelsman B, Coetzee D, Valtr L, Verbecque E. Do Girls Have an Advantage Compared to Boys When Their Motor Skills Are Tested Using the Movement Assessment Battery for Children, 2nd Edition? Children (Basel). 2023;10(7):1159.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Navarro-Patón R, Lago-Ballesteros J, Arufe-Giráldez V, Sanmiguel-Rodríguez A, Lago-Fuentes C, Mecías-Calvo M. Gender differences on motor competence in 5-year-old preschool children regarding relative age. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(6):3143.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  37. Mecías-Calvo M, Arufe-Giráldez V, Cons-Ferreiro M, Navarro-Patón R. Is it possible to reduce the relative age effect through an intervention on motor competence in preschool children? Children (Basel). 2021;8(5):386.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  38. Zheng Y, Ye W, Korivi M, Liu Y, Hong F. Gender differences in fundamental motor skills proficiency in children aged 3–6 years: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2022;19(14): 8318.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  39. Honrubia-Montesinos C, Gil-Madrona P, Losada-Puente L. Motor development among spanish preschool children. Children. 2021;8: 41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Barnett LM, Van Beurden E, Morgan PJ, Brooks LO, Beard JR. Gender differences in motor skill proficiency from childhood to adolescence: a longitudinal study. Res Q Exerc Sport. 2010;81(2):162–70.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Robinson LE. The Relationship between perceived physical competence and fundamental motor skills in preschool children. Child Care Health Dev. 2011;37(4):589–96.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Bakhtiar S. Fundamental motor skill among 6-year-old children in Padang, West Sumatera, Indonesia. Asian Soc Sci. 2014;10:155–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  43. Singh DK, Rahman NN, Rajikan R, Zainudin A, Nordin NA, Karim ZA, Yee YH. Balance and motor skills among preschool children aged 3 to 4 years old. Malaysian J Med Health Sci. 2015;11:63–8.

    Google Scholar 

  44. Cameron CE, Brock LL, Murrah WM, Bell LH, Worzalla SL, Grissmer D, et al. Fine motor skills and executive function both contribute to kindergarten achievement. Child Dev. 2012;83(4):1229–44.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  45. Mierau A, Felsch M, Hülsdünker T, Mierau J, Bullermann P, Weiß B, et al. The interrelation between sensorimotor abilities, cognitive performance and individual EEG alpha peak frequency in young children. Clin Neurophysiol. 2016;127(1):270–6.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Oberer N, Gashaj V, Roebers CM. Motor skills in kindergarten: internal structure, cognitive correlates and relationships to background variables. Hum Mov Sci. 2017;52:170–80.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Stein M, Auerswald M, Ebersbach M. Relationships between motor and executive functions and the effect of an acute coordinative intervention on executive functions in kindergartners. Front Psychol. 2017;8: 859.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  48. Cook CJ, Howard SJ, Scerif G, Twine R, Kahn K, Norris SA, et al. Associations of physical activity and gross motor skills with executive function in preschool children from low-income South African settings. Dev Sci. 2019;22(5): e12820.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Albuquerque MR, Rennó GVC, Bruzi AT, Fortes LS, Malloy-Diniz LF. Association between motor competence and executive functions in children. Appl Neuropsychol Child. 2021;11(3):495–503.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  50. Gandotra A, Csaba S, Sattar Y, Cserényi R, Bizonics R, Cserjesi R, et al. A meta-analysis of the relationship between motor skills and executive functions in typically-developing children. J Cogn Dev. 2021;23(1):83–110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  51. Diamond A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. 2013;64:135–68.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Souissi S, Chamari K, Bellaj T. Assessment of executive functions in school-aged children: A narrative review. Front Psychol. 2022;13: 991699.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  53. Miyake A, Friedman NP. The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(1):8–14.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  54. Chan RC, Shum D, Toulopoulou T, Chen EY. Assessment of executive functions: Review of instruments and identification of critical issues. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2008;23:201–16.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Brock LL, Rimm-Kaufman SE, Nathanson L, Grimm KJ. The contributions of ‘hot’ and ‘cool’ executive function to children’s academic achievement, learning-related behaviors, and engagement in kindergarten. Early Childh Res. 2009;Q. 24:337–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Baggetta P, Alexander PA. Conceptualization and operationalization of executive function. Mind, Brain, and Educationю. 2016;10(1):10–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Hughes C, Ensor R, Wilson A, Graham A. Tracking executive function across the transition to school: A latent variable approach. Dev Neuropsychol. 2010;35:20–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Sambol S, Suleyman E, Scarfo J, Ball M. A true reflection of executive functioning or a representation of task-specific variance? Re-evaluating the unity/diversity framework. Acta Psychologica. 2023;236:236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  59. Goldstein S, Naglieri JA, editors. Handbook of Executive Functioning. New York, NY: Springer; 2014.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager T. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: a latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49–100.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Almazova O, Bukhalenkova D, Veraksa N. Assessment of the level of development of executive functions in the senior preschool age. Psycol J High School of Econ. 2019;16(2):302–17.

    Google Scholar 

  62. Robson DA, Allen MS, Howard SJ. Self-regulation in childhood as a predictor of future outcomes: a meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2020;146(4):324–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Scionti N, Cavallerom M, Zogmaister C, Marzocchi GM. Is cognitive training effective for improving executive functions in preschoolers? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Front Psychol. 2020;10:2812.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  64. Fomina TG, Bondarenko IN, Morosanova VI. Conscious self-regulation, school engagement and academic performance in adolescents: differential psychological aspect. RUDN J Psychol Pedagogics. 2023;20(3):560–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Lavrova MA, Kharitonova MP. Self-regulation style and quality of life in the elderly. Natl Psychol J. 2023;2(50):79–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Potanina AM, Morosanova VI. Individually-typological features of the relationship of conscious self-regulation, psychological well-being, and academic achievement in 6th grade students. New Ideas Child Educ Psychol. 2023;3(1–2):27–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  67. Zakharova MN, Machinskaya RI. Voluntary control of cognitive activity in preschool children: age-dependent changes from ages 3–4 to 4–5. Psychol Russ. 2023;16(3):122–31.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Veraksa AN, Gavrilova MN, Karimova AI, Solopova OV, Yakushina AA. Regularly functions in preschoolers. aged 4–7: the impact of kindergarten attendance span. Lomonosov Psychol J. 2023;46(4):64–87.

    Google Scholar 

  69. McClelland MM, Cameron CE. Developing together: the role of executive function and motor skills in children’s early academic lives. Early Child Res. 2019;46:142–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Fernandes AC, Viegas ÂA, Lacerda ACR, et al. Association between executive functions and gross motor skills in overweight/obese and eutrophic preschoolers: cross-sectional study. BMC Pediatr. 2022;22:498.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  71. Kim H, Duran CAK, Cameron CE, Grissmer D. Developmental relations among motor and cognitive processes and mathematics skills. Child Dev. 2018;89(2):476–94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Maurer MN, Roebers CM. Towards a better understanding of the association between motor skills and executive functions in 5- to 6-year-olds: the impact of motor task difficulty. Hum Mov Sci. 2019;66:607–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Diamond A. Close interrelation of motor development and cognitive development and of the cerebellum and prefrontal cortex. Child Dev. 2000;71(1):44–56.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Roebers CM, Röthlisberger M, Neuenschwander R, Cimeli P, Michel E, Jäger K. The relation between cognitive and motor performance and their relevance for children’s transition to school: a latent variable approach. Hum Mov Sci. 2014;33:284–97.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Korkman M, Kirk U, Kemp SL. NEPSY. Administrative manual. 2nd ed. San Antonio: Psychological Corporation; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  76. Zelazo PD. The Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS): a method of assessing executive function in children. Natl Protoc. 2006;1(1):297–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Vrantsidis DM, Clark CAC, Chevalier N, Espy KA, Wiebe SA. Socioeconomic status and executive function in early childhood: exploring proximal mechanisms. Dev Sci. 2020;23(3): e12917.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  78. Veraksa A, Almazova O, Bukhalenkova D. Executive functions assessment in senior preschool age: a battery of methods. Psychol J. 2020;41:108–18.

    Google Scholar 

  79. Veraksa NE, Aslanova MS, Tarasova KS, Klimenko VA. Technique for diagnosing cognitive flexibility in preschoolers: comparison of blank and digital forms. RUDN J Psychol Pedagogics. 2023;20(1):105–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Dancey CP, Reidy J. Statistics without maths for psychology. London: Pearson Education; 2007.

  81. López-Martín E, Ardura-Martínez D. The effect size in scientific publication. Educación XX1. 2023;26(1):9–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. Cohen J. Statistical power analysis for the behavioural sciences. 2nd ed. New York: Academic Press; 1988.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Dovolnova IV. The specifics of the manifestations of initiative by boys and girls of preschool and primary school age in the socio-cultural environment. Preschool Educ Today. 2023;1:54–64.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Gavrilova MN, Chichinina EA, Yakushina AA. Assessment of motor development in preschool age: A review of assessment tools. Russ Psychol J. 2023;20(4):293–314.

    Google Scholar 

  85. Stöckel T, Hughes CML. The relation between measures of cognitive and motor functioning in 5- to 6-year old children. Psychol Res. 2016;80(4):543–4.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Niederer I, Kriemler S, Gut J, Hartmann T, Schindler C, Barral J, et al. Relationship of aerobic fitness and motor skills with memory and attention in preschoolers (Ballabeina): a cross-sectional and longitudinal study. BMC Pediatr. 2011;11(1):1–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Wassenberg R, Feron F, Kessels A, Hendriksen J, Kalff A, Kroes M, et al. Relation between cognitive and motor performance in 5- to 6-year-old children: results from a large-scale cross-sectional study. Child Dev. 2005;76(5):1092–103.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  88. Van Der Veer G, Kamphorst E, Cantell M, Minnaert A, Houwen S. Task-specific and latent relationships between motor skills and executive functions in preschool children. Front Psychol. 2020;11: 2208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  89. Bruininks RH, Bruininks BD. Bruininks-Oseretsky test of motor proficiency. 2nd ed. Pearson Assessments; 2005.

  90. Saraiva L, Rodrigues LP, Cordovil R, Barreiros J. Motor profile of Portuguese preschool children on the Peabody Developmental Motor Scales-2: a cross-cultural study. Res Dev Disabil. 2013;34(6):1966–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Not applicable.

Funding

This research was funded by Russian Science Foundation grant number 23–78-30005.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

Conceptualization, MG; methodology, EC, MG; writing-original draft preparation, EC; writing-review and editing MG and PD; All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Elena Chichinina or Patrik Drid.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in the study involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. This study and its consent procedures were approved by the Ethics Committee of Faculty of Psychology at Lomonosov Moscow State University (the approval No: 2023/18). All caregivers provided written informed consent for their child’s participation in the study.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License, which permits any non-commercial use, sharing, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if you modified the licensed material. You do not have permission under this licence to share adapted material derived from this article or parts of it. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Chichinina, E., Gavrilova, M. & Drid, P. Executive function skills predict motor competence in preschool children. BMC Pediatr 25, 235 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12887-025-05582-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12887-025-05582-z

Keywords