- Research
- Open access
- Published:
Is maternal diabetes during pregnancy associated with neurodevelopmental, cognitive and behavioural outcomes in children? Insights from individual participant data meta-analysis in ten birth cohorts
BMC Pediatrics volume 25, Article number: 76 (2025)
Abstract
Background
Growing evidence shows that dysregulated metabolic intrauterine environments can affect offspring’s neurodevelopment and behaviour. However, the results of individual cohort studies have been inconsistent. We aimed to investigate the association between maternal diabetes before pregnancy and gestational diabetes mellitus (GDM) with neurodevelopmental, cognitive and behavioural outcomes in children.
Methods
Harmonised data from > 200 000 mother-child pairs across ten birth cohorts in Europe and Australia were available. Mother-child pairs were included for analysis to determine whether GDM was recorded (yes or no) and whether at least one neurodevelopmental, cognitive and behavioural outcome was available in children aged 3 to 13 years. Confounder-adjusted regression models were used to estimate associations between maternal diabetes and child outcomes using two-stage individual participant data (IPD) meta-analysis. Model 1 included a crude estimate. The full adjustment model (model 2) included adjustment for child sex, maternal age, pre-pregnancy BMI, pregnancy weight gain, maternal smoking during pregnancy, plurality, parity and maternal education.
Results
Children (aged 7–10 years) born to mothers with GDM had higher attention-deficient hyperactive disorder (ADHD) symptoms compared to non-exposed controls (model 2, regression coefficient (β) 3.67 (95% CI 1.13, 6.20), P = 0.001). Moreover, children (aged 4–6 years) born to mothers with GDM exhibited more externalising problems than those born to mothers without GDM (model 2, β 2.77 (95% CI 0.52, 5.02), P = 0.01). A pre-existing maternal history of type 1 and type 2 diabetes mellitus was associated with ADHD symptoms at 4–6 years (model 1, β 8.82 (95% CI 2.21, 15.45, P = 0.009) and β 7.90 (95% CI 0.82, 14.98, P = 0.02), respectively). The association was no longer apparent in further adjustments.
Conclusions
This study found that children between 4 - 6 and 7–10 years of age born to mothers with GDM have a greater likelihood of developing externalising problems and ADHD symptoms, respectively. Externalising problems often co-exist with ADHD symptoms and precede formal ADHD diagnosis. Overall, this large-scale multi-cohort study suggested that a dysregulated metabolic environment during pregnancy may contribute to ADHD symptoms and externalising problems in young children.
Introduction
Between 10% and 20% of children are affected annually by mental health, cognitive and behavioural disorders, with similar rates across different racial and ethnic groups after controlling for income, resident status, education, and neighbourhood support [1, 2]. This can manifest as internalising or externalising problems, delayed non-verbal intelligence, language, and gross and fine motor development in childhood. Specific neurodevelopmental disorders such as autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) are prevalent worldwide, affecting approximately 2.9% and 8.5% of children, respectively [3, 4]. ADHD is characterised by symptoms such as inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity and often co-occurs with ASD, which is characterised by a lack of social interaction as well as restrictive, repetitive patterns of behaviour, interest or activities [3, 5].
It is becoming increasingly evident that the in-utero metabolic milieu may impact brain development in offspring [6,7,8,9,10]. Exposure to diabetes before or during pregnancy, characterised by elevated blood glucose levels and placental inflammation responses, may impact early fetal brain development and result in delayed brain maturation [9, 11,12,13,14,15,16]. Type 1 and Type 2 diabetes mellitus (T1DM and T2DM) affects 529 million women worldwide [17], whereas diabetes during pregnancy (gestational diabetes mellitus (GDM)) affects approximately one in six women (17.9%) [18,19,20].
Xiang and colleagues (2023) studied the relation between maternal diabetes in pregnancy and neurodevelopmental disorders (ASD [12] and ADHD [21]) in offspring using health record data (n > 300 000) of children born at Kaiser Permanente Southern California (KPSC) from 1995 to 2009 with follow-up until 2012. No overall association was found between GDM and either ASD [21] or ADHD [34]. Timing and dose of exposure to GDM were explored. Timing appeared to play a role in the association with ASD but not with ADHD. An early GDM diagnosis (before 26 weeks gestation) was associated with an increased risk of ASD diagnosis [12, 21]. By contrast, no association was found between the timing of GDM exposure and ADHD [21]. Evidence for a dose response was also suggested. A higher maternal haemoglobin A1c (HbA1c) level in early pregnancy was associated with an increased risk of ASD in the offspring, suggesting that glycaemic control in early pregnancy may be an important window for ASD risk in offspring [22]. In addition, a significant association was detected between mothers with GDM taking antidiabetic medication and ADHD in their children. No such association was present in the mothers with GDM not receiving medication [21].
Previous studies have also examined the relationship between maternal diabetes and cognitive and behavioural problems, such as motor development, intelligence, and internalising and externalising behaviours, with varying results [23,24,25]. In a meta-analysis by Arabiat et al. (2021) [23], it was found that children born to mothers with diabetes (during pregnancy and pre-existing) scored lower on tests of gross motor function compared with children born to mothers without diabetes. This study reported the weighted mean difference as − 0.75 (95% CI: − 1.29, − 0.21), with a p-value of 0.007 and an I2 of 24%. It’s important to note that the study did not separately analyse the association of T1DM, T2DM, and GDM with gross motor function. More recently, Faleschini et al. (2023) [37] studied 548 mother-child pairs from a prospective pre-birth Gen3G cohort in Canada and measured maternal glycaemic markers during pregnancy using an oral glucose tolerance test (OGTT). The authors report that exposure to GDM was associated with higher externalising scores at 3 and 5 years [β = 1.12, 95% CI: 0.14, 2.10] after adjustment for child sex, maternal body mass index and family history of diabetes [25]. The authors suggested an association between exposure to maternal GDM during pregnancy and greater levels of externalising behaviours in young children (3–5 years).
Given the variability in findings, longer follow-ups across the childhood period within large numbers are needed to evaluate if the associations persist or translate into other related outcomes. Well-designed international meta-analyses are required. Data linkage using healthcare data has used this approach, finding a small to moderate association between maternal diabetes mellitus and ADHD [26]. Our approach is to use two-stage individual participant data (IPD) meta-analysis applied on harmonised data, which provides reliable regression estimates by reducing between-study heterogeneity and allowing consistent adjustment for confounding factors [27]. Therefore, we aimed to use data from more than 200,000 mother-child dyads from ten different cohorts participating in the European Union Child Cohort Network (EUCCN) with rigorous harmonisation of GDM and mental health outcomes to investigate the association between diabetes before and during pregnancy and its potential impact on neurodevelopmental, cognitive, and behavioural outcomes in children between 3 and 13 years of age.
Method
Cohort studies and harmonisation of core variables
The study was part of the European Union-funded Horizon 2020 Project LifeCycle, with harmonised data from the EUCCN, an international collaboration between Australian and European birth cohort studies [28, 29]. The trial registration number for the project is ECCNLC202161 and the work was supported by funding from the Horizon 2020 LifeCycle (733206). The LifeCycle project has developed a protocol to generate with this aised variables across collected variables. Details of how variables were harmonised for LifeCycle are provided in a publicly available online catalogue (https://euchildcohortnetwork.eu/research-tools/) and elsewhere [28]. Jaddoe et al. 2020 [29] fully describe the work to achieve a harmonised set of FAIR (findable, accessible, interoperable, and reusable) data resources known as the EU Child Cohort Network (EUCCN).
Pregnancy and birth cohort studies from the EUCCN were eligible to participate if they had data on maternal GDM diagnosis (Yes or No) collected during pregnancy continuing beyond 24–28 weeks of gestation and data on at least one child’s neurodevelopmental, cognitive or behavioural outcome. Ten cohorts were eligible to participate in the study, and all agreed to participate in this analysis. These were ALSPAC (Avon Longitudinal Study of Parents and Children, United Kingdom, 1991–1992) [30, 31], BiB (Born in Bradford, United Kingdom, 2007–2011) [32], DNBC (Danish National Birth Cohort, Denmark, 1996–2003) [33], EDEN (study on the pre- & early postnatal determinants of child health & development, France, 2003–2006) [34], ELFE (The French National cohort of children, France, 2011–2016) [35], GenR (The Generation R Study, Rotterdam, the Netherlands, 2002–2006) [36], INMA (Environment and Childhood Project, Spain, 1997–2008) [37], MoBa (The Norwegian Mother, Father and Child Cohort Study, Norway, 1999–2008) [38], NINFEA (Nascita e INFanzia: gli Effetti dell’Ambiente, Italy, 2005–2016) [39] and Raine (The Raine Study, Australia, 1989–1991) [40]. All studies had ethical approval and obtained parental or participant written informed consent (Supplementary text 1).
Exposures: maternal diabetes before and during pregnancy
Our primary exposure measure was a binary variable indicating the presence or absence of evidence for GDM. A secondary analysis was also performed using data about T1DM and T2DM before pregnancy. GDM, T1DM and T2DM were extracted from medical records, blood samples, OGTT, or maternal self-reporting results in questionnaires. Although, there is variability in ascertainment methods, each cohort harmonised their data according to the consortium’s protocol into a common data model format [28]. A binary variable (Yes or No) indicating the presence or absence of evidence for GDM was harmonised for each cohort based on extraction from clinical records or maternal self-report (Supplemental Table 1).
Outcomes: child neurodevelopmental, cognitive and behavioural outcomes
We analysed data for seven neurodevelopmental and cognitive behavioural outcomes: ADHD symptoms, ASD symptoms, gross motor function, fine motor development, non-verbal intelligence, internalising, and externalising behaviours. These were core variables rigorously harmonised by strict protocols. A full list of the neurodevelopmental and cognitive behavioural outcomes harmonised in the EUCCN can be found in Work Package 6 of LifeCycle (https://euchildcohortnetwork.eu/research-tools/).
This study grouped outcomes in four age ranges: 3 years, 4–6 years, 7–10 years, and 11–13 years, broadly representing the stages of toddler preschool, school entry, late childhood and early adolescence. Age groups were selected to maximize the use of the available data from the various follow-up points while simultaneously considering childhood developmental stages and mirror prior analyses on these data [41].
A recent publication by Nader et al. 2023 [42] provides a detailed overview of the major mental health measures available in the LifeCycle project. ADHD percentile scores were measured using the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) [43], Revised Conners’ Parent Rating Scale (CPRS-R) [44], Diagnostic Interview Schedule for Children (DISC-IV/DSM) [45], Teacher’s Report Form (TRF) [46] and Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviour (SWAN) [47]. ASD was measured by several instruments and medical records including the Alarm Distress Baby Scale (ADBB) [48], Autism Quotient Questionnaire (AQ) [49], Childhood Autism Spectrum Test (CAST) [50], Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) [51], the Early Screening of Autistic Traits Questionnaire (ESAT) [52], the Non-Verbal Communication Checklist (NVCC) [53], and the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ) [54].
Gross and fine motor function were assessed using various instruments across the cohorts. They included the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ) [55], Peg Moving Task (PMT) [56], Brunet-Lezine psychometric scale (BDIST) [57], Bayley Scale of Infant Development (BSID) [58], Child Development Inventory (CDI) [59], Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (DCDQ) [60], Denver Development Screening Test (DDST) [61], Movement Assessment Battery for Children (M-ABC) [62], Children’s developmental progress from birth to five years (STYCAR) [63], McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA) [64].
Internalising and externalising problems were measured using Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL) [65], Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ) [66]. Measurement tools used to measure nonverbal intelligence included Ages and Stages questionnaires (ASQ) [55, 67], British Ability Scale (BAS) [68], Snijders-Oomen Non-verbal Intelligence Test (SON-R) [69], Bayley Scale of Infant Development (BSID) [58], Culture Fair Intelligence Test (CFIT) [70], Cartell Infant Intelligence Scale (CIIS) [71], McCarthy Scales of Children’s Abilities (MSCA) [64], Snijders-Oomen Non-Verbal Intelligence Test (SON-R) [69], Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC) [72].
Table 1 shows which cohorts have data on neurodevelopmental, cognitive, and behavioural outcomes (measured as percentile scores) for each age group and the number of mother-child pairs included in each case.
Percentile scores were calculated for each cohort and data collection wave separately to compare the outcomes on the same scale (rather than the original scale of the different instruments) [42, 73]. A percentile score indicates a child’s relative position within his/her cohort and age group [42]. The harmonisation process under the LifeCycle project allows meta-analysis of data initially collected using different scales or instruments [28].
Confounders
Potential confounders were identified based on the literature [74,75,76] and these measures were harmonised across cohorts. Two models of estimates were used. Model 1 was crude and model 2 was adjusted for the following confounders: maternal gestational weight, pre-pregnancy body mass index (BMI), maternal smoking during pregnancy, parity (number of times giving birth), plurality, maternal education and household income. Information on the confounders, including child sex and maternal smoking during pregnancy, was obtained from hospital records and/or questionnaires. Maternal pre-pregnancy BMI was determined by weight and height at the first visit. Maternal education variable was harmonised across cohorts based on the International Standard Classification of Education 97 (ISCED-97) and consisted of three categories: Low (No education to lower secondary; ISCED‐97 categories 0‐2), Medium (Upper and post‐secondary; ISCED‐97 categories 3‐4), High (Degree and above; ISCED‐97 categories 5‐6) [76, 77]. For the sensitivity adjustment (model 3), the EU statistics on income and living conditions (EU-SILC) were added to collect timely and comparable cross-sectional and longitudinal data on income, poverty, social exclusion, and living conditions [76]. The Raine Study was excluded from analysis model 3 due to the absence of EU-SILC income data.
Statistical analysis
The two-stage IPD meta-analysis examined the relationship between GDM and neurodevelopmental, cognitive and behavioural outcomes in children aged 3, 4–6, 7–10, and 11–13 years. A regression model is fitted on the data of each cohort separately and the cohort-specific estimates are then combined with a random-effects meta-analysis. We used the rma function from the metafor R package (version 4.6-0) with the Restricted Estimate Maximum Likelihood method for the random-effects meta-analysis. With this method, the combined estimate is given as the weighted average of the cohort-specific estimates where the weights are defined as the inverse of the variance of the estimates.
We employed linear regression models since percentile scores were used for the outcome variables. Each regression was performed on the complete cases for each set of variables (exposure, outcome, and confounders). Therefore, data about children with at least one missing value for any variable included in a model were excluded from the analysis. Regression models were fitted separately for each cohort, and regression coefficients (β) and standard errors (SE) were combined using random effects meta-analysis with the restricted maximum likelihood estimator method [78]. Between-cohort heterogeneity was evaluated by I2 and Q statistics. In the secondary analysis, we examined the associations of maternal T1DM and T2DM diabetes before pregnancy with percentile scores of ADHD symptoms.
All analyses were run on DataSHIELD (R packages dsBaseClient v6.1.0 & dsHelper v1.1.0), a platform allowing privacy-preserving co-analysis of data from multiple cohorts without the need to share or transfer the individual-level data [79, 80].
Results
Table 2 summarises the maternal characteristics of each cohort. Mean maternal age ranged between 27 and 33 years. Mean maternal BMI was 23.2 kg/m2. EDEN, ELFE, and MoBa had the highest proportion of mothers with higher education, at 53.5%, 56.6%, and 64.2%, respectively. Raine study, ALSPAC and BiB had a lower proportion of mothers with higher education, at 19.4%, 13.2% and 27.6% respectively. A high rate of vaginal birth was observed among the cohorts, the exceptions being MoBa, BiB and The Raine study having rates below 70%.
In total, 266 970 pregnant women across the ten cohorts had information on the presence or absence of GDM. The prevalence of GDM differed between cohorts, ranging from 0.65% (NINFEA) to 8.01% (BiB) (Table 2). The prevalence of T1DM ranged from 0.003% (NINFEA) to 0.24% (ALSPAC), and of T2DM ranged from 0.00% (NINFEA) to 0.25% (ELFE). A total of 132,249 pregnant mothers across four cohorts (ALSPAC, BiB, DNBC, and ELFE) had data available on maternal T1DM and T2DM before pregnancy. Overall, the distribution of birth weight and head circumference were similar across the cohorts, with a combined median of 3371 g (interquartile range (IQR): 3038, 3855) for birth weight and 33.88 cm (IQR: 34.86, 35.88) for head circumference at birth.
Association of GDM with neurodevelopmental, cognitive and behavioural outcomes
Table 3 shows the adjusted regression estimates (β) of the association between GDM and neurodevelopment, cognitive and behavioural outcomes (measured as percentile scores) at different ages derived from the two-stage IPD meta-analysis.
GDM and ADHD
In the crude estimates (models 1), children aged 4–6 years born to mothers with GDM had significantly higher percentile scores of ADHD symptoms than those born to mothers without GDM (β =2.65 (95% CI:0.87, 4.44) P = 0.004). After full adjustment (model 2), the associations retain significance (β =2.96 (95% CI:1.10, 4.81) P = 0.001). The association was no longer apparent after the sensitivity analysis (model 3) (β =1.65 (95% CI: −0.27, 3.58) P = 0.09). Notably, significance remained unchanged in children aged 7–10 years in crude (model 1) and adjusted estimates (model 2 and 3), demonstrating that children born to mothers with GDM had significantly higher percentile scores of ADHD symptoms than children born to mothers without GDM (model 1: β =4.09 (95% CI: 01.97, 6.20) (P < 0.001), (β =3.67 (95% CI: 1.13, 6.20) P = 0.001)and model 3: β = 2.40 (95% CI: 0.07, 4.73) P = 0.04). The I2 was 0.00% for the three adjusted models at both age groups (4–6 and 7–10 years) (Fig. 1). There was no significant association between GDM and ADHD in children between 11 and 13 years of age.
Association between GDM and offspring’s ADHD symptoms at 4–6 and 7–10 year of age. The forest plot shows the Regression Coefficient (β) and random effect (RE) for ADHD percentiles. Model 1 (a) and (d) include crude estimates, model 2 (b) and (e) full adjustment for child sex, maternal age, plurality and parity, BMI, pregnancy weight gain, and maternal smoking, and sensitivity adjustments (model 3 (c) and (f)) include full adjustment and EU-SILC. The Sensitivity adjustment excludes the Raine Study
GDM and ASD
In the crude estimates (model 1) mothers with GDM tend to have children who exhibit more ASD symptoms at ages 4–6 and 7–10 compared to mothers without GDM (β = 6.09 (95% CI: 1.03, 11.15) P = 0.01) and (β = 4.42 (95% CI: 0.11, 8.73) P = 0.04), respectively. After full adjustment (model 2) and sensitivity analysis (model 3), the associations diminished in children 4–6 years.
GDM and other neurodevelopmental, cognitive and behavioural symptoms
GDM was not significantly associated with any changes in motor function (gross and fine) and nonverbal intelligence in children of any age in all three adjusted models (Table 3). However, children aged 4–6 years born to mothers with GDM consistently exhibited more externalising problems than those born to mothers without GDM in crude estimates (model 1) (β =2.62 (95% CI: 0.51, 4.73) P = 0.01), full adjustment (model 2) (β =2.77 (95% CI: 0.52, 5.02) P = 0.01) sensitivity analysis (model 3) (β =2.50 (95% CI: 0.15, 4.85) P = 0.03). Low heterogeneity was present among the cohorts for this outcome (range I2: 0.00–3.4%) (Fig. 2). Externalising problems were present among children aged 7–10 years born to mothers with GDM in the crude estimates (model 1) (β 3.84 (95% CI 1.19, 6.49) P = 0.005). However, the association was no longer apparent after full adjustment (model 2) and sensitivity adjustment (model 3). There was no association between externalising problems in children (11–13 years old) and GDM. Similarly, GDM was associated with more internalising problems among children 7–10 years in the crude estimates (model 1) (β =5.65 (95% CI: 2.81, 8.50) P < 0.001). However, like externalising problems, the association diminished after further adjustment (models 2 and 3) for children between 7 and 10 years. For 11–13-year-old children born to mothers with GDM also showed significantly more internalising problems compared to children born to mothers without GDM in the crude estimates (model 1)(β =5.65 (95% CI: 0.40, 11.10) P = 0.03). However, the association was no longer apparent after further adjustments (models 2 and 3) (Table 3).
Association between GDM and offspring’s externalising problems at 4-6 and 7-10 year of age. The forest plot is showing Regression Coefficient (β) and random effect (RE) for externalising problems in children 4–6 and 7–10 years of age exposed to GDM versus children not exposed to GDM. Model 1 (a) and (d) include crude estimates, model 2 (b) and (e) full adjustment for child sex, maternal age, plurality and parity, BMI, pregnancy weight gain, maternal smoking and sensitivity adjustments (model 3 (c) and (f)) include full adjustment and EU-SILC. The Sensitivity adjustment excludes the Raine Study
In a secondary analysis, the association between maternal diabetes before pregnancy (excluding GDM) and child ADHD symptoms was examined. In the crude estimates (model 1) children born to mothers with T1DM and T2DM had more significant ADHD symptoms than their counterparts at 4–6 years of age compared to the children born to mothers without diabetes before pregnancy (model 1) ((β =8.82 (95% CI: 2.21, 15.42) P = 0.009) and (β = 7.90 (95% CI: 0.82, 14.98) P = 0.02 )) (Table 4). The effects diminished after full adjustment (models 2) for both T1DM (β = 4.33 (95% CI −7.91, 16.56) P = 0.48) and T2DM (β = 6.50 (95% CI: −0.89, 13.90) P = 0.08) and after sensitivity analysis (model 3) (β = 4.33 (95% CI: −7.83, 16.49) P = 0.48) and T2DM (β = 6.27 (95% CI: −4.60, 17.15) P = 0.25). There was no association between GDM and ADHD in any adjusted models in children aged 7–10. In Model 2, the heterogeneity increased in both age groups compared to model 1 (Fig. 3).
Association between T1DM and T2DM and offspring’s ADHD symptoms at 4–6 year of age. Forest plot showing Regression Coefficient (β) and random effect (RE) for ADHD percentile in children (4–6 years)) exposed to (1) T1DM and (2) T2DM versus those not exposed to T1DM and T2DM, respectively. Model 1 (a) and (d) include crude estimates, model 2 (b) and (e) full adjustment for child sex, maternal age, plurality and parity, BMI, pregnancy weight gain, maternal smoking and sensitivity adjustments (model 3 (c) and (f)) include full adjustment and EU-SILC. The Sensitivity adjustment excludes the Raine Study
Discussion
In this study, a consistent finding in the crude and fully adjusted models was that children aged 7–10 years who were exposed to GDM had more ADHD symptoms than children born to mothers without GDM. We also found that children between the ages of 4–6 years born to mothers diagnosed with GDM exhibited higher externalising problems compared to those born to mothers without GDM after adjustments. The results suggest that GDM may be linked to ADHD symptoms in older children as well as externalising problems that often co-occur with ADHD in younger children. This is the first and the largest IPD meta-analysis using harmonised individual-level data that has investigated the association between maternal diabetes before and during pregnancy with neurodevelopmental, cognitive and behavioural outcomes in children from 3 years up to 13 years.
These results corroborate the American data from the Kaiser Permanente study [12, 21] which showed that a significant association was detected between some mothers with GDM and ADHD in their children, with the association being restricted to mothers with GDM taking antidiabetic medication. Our study has consolidated this finding and extended this more generally, such that the association persists among all women with GDM. This confirms the findings from another recent multinational meta-analysis [26], further confirming the robust association between GDM and ADHD.
Cognitive, emotional, and behavioural difficulties first emerge in early childhood, laying the foundation for continued or increasing problems during middle and late childhood [81]. Still, the mechanisms underlying these longitudinal associations or co-development between neurodevelopment, cognitive and behavioural domains from early childhood to early adulthood remain poorly elucidated [82]. Our finding that GDM is associated with externalising outcomes in young children (4–6 years) is consistent with the results of a previous study by Faleschini et al. [25], who found an association between GDM and externalising behaviours in young children at age 3 and 5 years. Interestingly, while we tested the associations at different age ranges, our significant finding was at the same approximate age as the Canadian study, around 3–6 years, suggesting that this is a sensitive age range for detecting this childhood symptomatology. ADHD does not have biological markers for diagnosis, making ADHD a disorder that is difficult to detect before symptoms manifest [83, 84]. However, our findings suggest that these externalising behaviours co-develop and extend into other domains, such as ADHD symptoms. We postulate that children may exhibit more externalising problems at younger ages and that as they mature, symptoms or behaviour related to ADHD may become more apparent.
Of note, our findings were attenuated with adjustment, particularly with a measure of the family’s socio-economic status. Nomura et al. (2012) [85] reported that children exposed to both GDM, and low socioeconomic status had a 14-fold increase in the risk for ADHD compared to those exposed GDM or low socioeconomic status (SES) alone. More recently, Cadman et al. (2024) [86]. Showed in their longitudinal study that children born into more disadvantaged socioeconomic status had more behavioural and cognitive problems. While women with low socioeconomic status are more likely to have more severe hyperglycaemia, which may affect the neurodevelopment of their children, household SES may also reflect poorer diet, greater maternal and child obesity, poorer health literacy, lesser early educational opportunities or other unmeasured factors. Recent studies show that siblings with discordant exposure to GDM in pregnancy had a similar risk of ADHD [26]. Indeed, such factors and shared genetics or familial factors between mother and offspring may partially or fully confound the association.
Increased inflammation, oxidative stress, hypoxia, and hyperinsulinemia during pregnancy may influence certain pathways in a child’s brain programming in-utero and contribute to neurodevelopmental, cognitive and behavioural outcomes later in life [21, 87,88,89]. Several studies suggest that maternal obesity, chronic inflammation, and maternal diabetes have a joint impact on the development of ASD and ADHD in children, which is greater than the impact of either condition alone [11, 90,91,92,93]. Additionally, it has been observed that the extent of diabetes (T1DM vs. T2DM vs. GDM requiring antidiabetic medication due to severe hyperglycaemia) during pregnancy has a more significant impact on the risk of ADHD symptoms. On the other hand, the timing of maternal diabetes does not influence ADHD symptoms [21].
This is a large two-staged meta-analysis using IPD to examine associations between maternal diabetes before and during pregnancy and various determinants of neurodevelopmental and cognitive and behavioural outcomes in children at various age groups. Our study has the following strengths. First, two-stage meta-analysis of IPD has advantages over the meta-analysis of published aggregate data as it avoids potential publication bias and reduces between-study heterogeneity by using harmonised data [94]. Second, the harmonised data on GDM diagnosis, neurodevelopmental, and cognitive and behavioural outcomes ensured comparability across cohorts. Third, the federated analysis using the DataSHIELD ensures that all analysis is performed identically, eliminating the need for individual researchers within each cohort to run analysis scripts. Fourth, combining data from 10 cohorts leads to larger numbers, providing the opportunity to increase statistical powder and obtain more precise estimates than any single cohort [86, 95]. Finally, replicating findings across diverse populations with varying cultural and socio-economic backgrounds enhances our confidence that the findings are applicable to a broader demographic, reinforcing the generalizability [28].
Using data from the EUCCN provided opportunities but also challenges. Our study has certain limitations. First, while GDM data were rigorously harmonised, the method of ascertainment varied (as reported in Supplementary Table 1). We particularly acknowledge the risk of bias when using parent-reported measures of cognitive and behavioural outcomes, which may have the potential for overestimation or proxy-reporting bias. Nevertheless, research suggests that parent-reported cognitive abilities can effectively assess cognitive and behavioural function when formal assessments are unavailable [96]. Attrition is a limitation in all studies that collect data over the years, and some cohorts may lack the data required to harmonise variables. However, in our study, we employed a complete case analysis to ensure that the results were not biased. Finally, confounding due to potentially unmeasured factors (such as factors related to SES and maternal ADHD) could not be ruled out.
To progress further in the field of child psychiatry and obstetrics health, it is crucial to understand better the relationship between maternal glucose levels during pregnancy (specifically, the severity of hyperglycaemia) and its impact on child brain development. Furthermore, the amelioration of significance with adjustment in our study also highlights a need for future studies to elucidate the relationships between factors, such as shared genetics, household income, poorer diet quality, greater obesity, poorer health literacy, lesser educational opportunities and child brain development.
Conclusion
Metabolic disorders, such as diabetes mellitus before and during pregnancy, are significant public health concerns that pose short and long-term health risks for both the mother and her child. Our study contributes to current knowledge and suggests a possible association between maternal diabetes during pregnancy, externalizing problems in young children aged 4–6 years and ADHD symptoms in children aged 7–10 years. Future research efforts should focus on better understanding the impact of severe metabolic dysregulation before and during pregnancy on early child brain development, to provide insights to improve mother and child health and well-being.
Data availability
No datasets were generated or analysed during the current study.
References
Ogundele MO. Behavioural and emotional disorders in childhood: a brief overview for paediatricians. World J Clin Pediatr. 2018;7(1):9–26.
Husky MM, et al. Self-reported mental health in children ages 6–12 years across eight European countries. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018;27(6):785–95.
Albajara Sáenz A, et al. Disorder-specific brain volumetric abnormalities in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder relative to autism spectrum disorder. PLoS One. 2020;15(11):e0241856.
Yang Y, et al. Prevalence of neurodevelopmental disorders among US children and adolescents in 2019 and 2020. Front Psychol. 2022;13:997648.https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.997648.
Battle DE. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM). Codas. 2013;25(2):191–2.
Peleg-Raibstein D. Understanding the link between maternal overnutrition, cardio-metabolic dysfunction and cognitive aging. Front Neurosci. 2021;15:645569.
Rowland J, Wilson CA. The association between gestational diabetes and ASD and ADHD: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Sci Rep. 2021;11(1):5136.
Denison FC, et al. Brain development in fetuses of mothers with diabetes: a case-control MR Imaging Study. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol. 2017;38(5):1037–44.
Rodolaki K, et al. The impact of maternal diabetes on the future health and neurodevelopment of the offspring: a review of the evidence. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). 2023;14:1125628.
Pantham P, Aye ILMH, Powell TL. Inflammation in maternal obesity and gestational diabetes mellitus. Placenta. 2015;36(7):709–15.
Xiang AH, et al. Maternal type 1 diabetes and risk of autism in offspring. JAMA. 2018;320(1):89–91.
Xiang AH, et al. Association of maternal diabetes with autism in offspring. JAMA. 2015;313(14):1425–34.
Zhao L, et al. The association of maternal diabetes with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder in offspring: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2019;15:675–84.
Carter SA, et al. Maternal obesity, diabetes, preeclampsia, and asthma during pregnancy and likelihood of autism spectrum disorder with gastrointestinal disturbances in offspring. Autism. 2023;27(4):916–26.
Xiang AH. Diabetes in pregnancy for mothers and offspring: reflection on 30 years of clinical and translational research: the 2022 Norbert Freinkel Award lecture. Diabetes Care. 2023;46(3):482–9.
Zhu B, et al. Gestational diabetes mellitus, autistic traits and ADHD symptoms in toddlers: placental inflammatory and oxidative stress cytokines do not play an intermediary role. Psychoneuroendocrinology. 2021;134:105435.
Ong KL, et al. Global, regional, and national burden of diabetes from 1990 to 2021, with projections of prevalence to 2050: a systematic analysis for the global burden of disease study 2021. Lancet. 2023;402(10397):203–34.
Kapur A, Seshiah V. Women & diabetes: our right to a healthy future. Indian J Med Res. 2017;146(5):553–6.
Ciarambino T, et al. Influence of gender in diabetes mellitus and its complication. Int J Mol Sci. 2022;23(16):8850.
Whincup PH, Kaye SJ, Owen CG, Huxley R, Cook DG, Anazawa S, et al. Birth weight and risk of type 2 diabetes: a systematic review. JAMA. 2008;300(24):2886–97. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1001/jama.2008.886.
Xiang AH, et al. Maternal gestational diabetes mellitus, type 1 diabetes, and type 2 diabetes during pregnancy and risk of ADHD in offspring. Diabetes Care. 2018;41(12):2502–8.
Xiang AH, et al. Hemoglobin A1c levels during pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorders in offspring. JAMA. 2019;322(5):460–1.
Arabiat D, et al. Motor developmental outcomes in children exposed to maternal diabetes during pregnancy: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2021;18(4):1699.
Yamamoto JM, et al. Neurocognitive and behavioural outcomes in offspring exposed to maternal pre-existing diabetes: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Diabetologia. 2019;62(9):1561–74.
Faleschini S, et al. Maternal hyperglycemia in pregnancy and offspring internalizing and externalizing behaviors. Matern Child Health J. 2023;27(10):1765–73.
Chan AYL, et al. Maternal diabetes and risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in offspring in a multinational cohort of 3.6 million mother–child pairs. Nat Med. 2024;30(5):1416–23.
Simmonds M, Stewart G, Stewart L. A decade of individual participant data meta-analyses: a review of current practice. Contemp Clin Trials. 2015;45:76–83.
de Pinot A, et al. The EU child cohort network’s core data: establishing a set of findable, accessible, interoperable and re-usable (FAIR) variables. Eur J Epidemiol. 2021;36(5):565–80.
Jaddoe VWV, et al. The LifeCycle project-EU child cohort network: a federated analysis infrastructure and harmonized data of more than 250,000 children and parents. Eur J Epidemiol. 2020;35(7):709–24.
Fraser A, et al. Cohort profile: the avon longitudinal study of parents and children: ALSPAC mothers cohort. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):97–110.
Boyd A, et al. Cohort profile: the ‘children of the 90s’--the index offspring of the avon longitudinal study of parents and children. Int J Epidemiol. 2013;42(1):111–27.
Wright J, et al. Cohort profile: the born in Bradford multi-ethnic family cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;42(4):978–91.
Olsen J, et al. The Danish national birth cohort–its background, structure and aim. Scand J Public Health. 2001;29(4):300–7.
Heude B, et al. Cohort profile: the EDEN mother-child cohort on the prenatal and early postnatal determinants of child health and development. Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45(2):353–63.
Charles MA, et al. Cohort profile: the French national cohort of children (ELFE): birth to 5 years. Int J Epidemiol. 2020;49(2):368–369j.
Kooijman MN, et al. The generation R Study: design and cohort update 2017. Eur J Epidemiol. 2016;31(12):1243–64.
Guxens M, et al. Cohort profile: the INMA—INfancia y Medio Ambiente—(environment and childhood) project. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;41(4):930–40.
Magnus P, et al. Cohort profile update: the Norwegian mother and child cohort study (MoBa). Int J Epidemiol. 2016;45(2):382–8.
Richiardi L, et al. Feasibility of recruiting a birth cohort through the internet: the experience of the NINFEA cohort. Eur J Epidemiol. 2007;22(12):831–7.
Straker L, et al. Cohort profile: the western Australian pregnancy cohort (Raine) study-generation 2. Int J Epidemiol. 2017;46(5):1384–1385j.
Barry KM, et al. Early childcare arrangements and children’s internalizing and externalizing symptoms: an individual participant data meta-analysis of six prospective birth cohorts in Europe. Lancet Reg Health Eur. 2024;45:101036.
Nader JL, et al. Measures of early-life behavior and later psychopathology in the LifeCycle project - EU child cohort network: a cohort description. J Epidemiol. 2023;33(6):321–31.
Achenbach TM. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4-18 and 1991 profile. Burlington: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry; 1991.
Conners CK, et al. The revised conners’ parent rating scale (CPRS-R): factor structure, reliability, and criterion validity. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 1998;26(4):257–68.
Shaffer D, et al. NIMH diagnostic interview schedule for children version IV (NIMH DISC-IV): description, differences from previous versions, and reliability of some common diagnoses. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2000;39(1):28–38.
Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms and Profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2001.
Swanson J. M, Schuck S, Porter M. M, Carlson C, Hartman C. A, Sergeant J. A, Clevenger W, Wasdell M, McCleary R, Lakes K, & Wigal T. Strengths and Weaknesses of ADHD Symptoms and Normal Behaviors Rating Scale (SWAN). 2012. APA PsycTests. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/t30027-000.
Guedeney A, Fermanian J. A validity and reliability study of assessment and screening for sustained withdrawal reaction in infancy: the alarm distress baby scale. Infant Mental Health J. 2001;22(5):559–75.
Auyeung B, et al. The autism spectrum quotient: children’s version (AQ-Child). J Autism Dev Disord. 2008;38(7):1230–40.
Scott FJ, et al. The CAST (childhood asperger syndrome test):preliminary development of a UK screen for mainstream primary-school-age children. Autism. 2002;6(1):9–31.
Constantino JN. Social Responsiveness Scale. In: Volkmar FR, editors. Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders. New York: Springer; 2013. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1007/978-1-4419-1698-3_296.
Swinkels SH, et al. Screening for autistic spectrum in children aged 14 to 15 months. I: the development of the early screening of autistic traits questionnaire (ESAT). J Autism Dev Disord. 2006;36(6):723–32.
Schjølberg S, et al. Predicting language development at age 18 months: data from the Norwegian mother and child cohort study. J Dev Behav Pediatrics: JDBP. 2011;32(5):375–83.
Beuker KT, et al. The structure of autism spectrum disorder symptoms in the general population at 18 months. J Autism Dev Disord. 2013;43(1):45–56.
Squires J, et al. Ages & stages questionnaires third edition ASQ-3 user’s guide. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing Co; 2009.
Annett M. Laterality of childhood hemiplegia and the growth of speech and intelligence. Cortex. 1973;9(1):4–33.
Brunet O, Lézine I. Desenvolvimento psicológico da primeira infância. Porto Alegre: Artes Médicas; 1981.
Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development 3rd Edition: Screening Test Manual. San Antonio: Harcourt Assessment, Inc.; 2006. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1037/t14978-000.
Ireton H, Glascoe FP. Assessing children’s development using parents’ reports. The child development inventory. Clin Pediatr (Phila). 1995;34(5):248–55.
Civetta LR, Hillier SL. The developmental coordination disorder questionnaire and movement assessment battery for children as a diagnostic method in Australian children. Pediatr Phys Ther. 2008;20(1):39–46.
Frankenburg WK, Dodds JB. The Denver developmental screening test. J Pediatr. 1967;71(2):181–91.
Geuze RH, et al. Clinical and research diagnostic criteria for developmental coordination disorder: a review and discussion. Hum Mov Sci. 2001;20(1):7–47.
Sheridan MD. Children’s developmental progress from birth to five years, the stycar sequences. Windsor: NFER; 1973.
Levin E. McCarthy scales of children’s abilities. In: Goldstein S, Naglieri JA, editors. Encyclopedia of child behavior and development. Boston: Springer US; 2011. p. 928–929.
Achenbach TM, Rescorla LA. Manual for the ASEBA School-Age Forms and Profiles. Burlington: University of Vermont Research Center for Children, Youth, & Families; 2001.
Goodman R. The strengths and difficulties Questionnaire: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38(5):581–6.
Sands R, D’Amato RC. McCarthy scales of children’s abilities. In: Kreutzer J, DeLuca J, Caplan B, editors. Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology. Cham: Springer International Publishing; 2017. p. 1–2.
Elliott CD, Smith P, McCulloch K. British ability scales. 2nd ed. 1996.
Jenkinson J, et al. Validation of the snijders-oomen nonverbal intelligence test - revised 2½-7 for Australian children with disabilities. J Psychoeducatio Assess. 1996;14(3):276–86.
Cattell RB, et al. Measuring intelligence with the culture fair tests. Champaign: Institute for Personality and Ability Testing; 1960.
Cattell P. The measurement of intelligence of infants and young children. Psychological Corporation; 1940.
Hedlund M. Manual for the McCarthy scales of children’s abilities. New York: The Psychological Corporation; 1972.
Fortier I, et al. Quality, quantity and harmony: the DataSHaPER approach to integrating data across bioclinical studies. Int J Epidemiol. 2010;39(5):1383–93.
Letourneau NL, et al. Socioeconomic status and child development: a meta-analysis. J Emot Behav Disorde. 2013;21(3):211–24.
Zhang H, et al. Parental and social factors in relation to child psychopathology, behavior, and cognitive function. Transl Psychiatry. 2020;10(1):80.
Eurostat. EU-SILC user data base description. 2009 2007. Available from: https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/web/microdata/european-union-statistics-on-income-and-living-conditions.
Pizzi C, et al. Measuring child socio-economic position in birth cohort research: the development of a novel standardized household income indicator. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(5):1700.
Tanriver-Ayder E, et al. Comparison of commonly used methods in random effects meta-analysis: application to preclinical data in drug discovery research. BMJ Open Sci. 2021;5(1):e100074.
Gaye A, et al. DataSHIELD: taking the analysis to the data, not the data to the analysis. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(6):1929–44.
Wilson RC, et al. DataSHIELD – new directions and dimensions. Data Sci J. 2017.
Zhang Q, Siman Liu, Zhengyan Wang, Nanhua Cheng, et al. Developmental cascades of behavior problems and cognitive ability from toddlerhood to middle childhood: a 9-year longitudinal study. Early Hum Dev. 2023;179:105731. https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2023.105731.
Kuja-Halkola R, et al. Codevelopment of ADHD and externalizing behavior from childhood to adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2015;56(6):640–7.
Yusuf Ali A, et al. Elements that influence the development of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) in children. Cureus. 2022;14(8):e27835.
Davidovitch M, et al. Challenges in defining the rates of ADHD diagnosis and treatment: trends over the last decade. BMC Pediatr. 2017;17(1):218.
Nomura Y, et al. Exposure to gestational diabetes mellitus and low socioeconomic status: effects on neurocognitive development and risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in offspring. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2012;166(4):337–43.
Cadman T, et al. Social inequalities in child mental health trajectories: a longitudinal study using birth cohort data 12 countries. BMC Public Health. 2024;24(1):2930.
Ronald A, Pennell CE, Whitehouse AJ. Prenatal maternal stress associated with ADHD and autistic traits in early childhood. Front Psychol. 2011;1:8023.
Van Lieshout RJ, Voruganti LP. Diabetes mellitus during pregnancy and increased risk of schizophrenia in offspring: a review of the evidence and putative mechanisms. J Psychiatry Neurosci. 2008;33(5):395–404.
Kong L, et al. Associations of different types of maternal diabetes and body mass index with offspring psychiatric disorders. JAMA Netw Open. 2020;3(2):e1920787–1920787.
Li L, et al. Maternal pre-pregnancy overweight/obesity and the risk of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in offspring: a systematic review, meta-analysis and quasi-experimental family-based study. Int J Epidemiol. 2020;49(3):857–75.
Kong L, et al. The risk of offspring psychiatric disorders in the setting of maternal obesity and diabetes. Pediatrics. 2018;142(3):e20180776.
Kong L, et al. Relationship of prenatal maternal obesity and diabetes to offspring neurodevelopmental and psychiatric disorders: a narrative review. Int J Obes (Lond). 2020;44(10):1981–2000.
Parisi F, et al. Maternal low-grade chronic inflammation and intrauterine programming of health and disease. Int J Mol Sci. 2021;22(4):1732.
Stewart LA, Tierney JF. To IPD or not to IPD? Advantages and disadvantages of systematic reviews using individual patient data. Eval Health Prof. 2002;25(1):76–97.
Larsen PS, et al. Pregnancy and birth cohort resources in Europe: a large opportunity for aetiological child health research. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2013;27(4):393–414.
Lee CM, et al. Agreement of parent-reported cognitive level with standardized measures among children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2023;16(6):1210–24.
Acknowledgements
We would like to thank all the mothers, fathers and children for generously donating their time and effort as participants in the cohorts included in LifeCycle. The long-term dedication and commitment of both the staff and participants involved in these studies have made this work possible. Cohort-specific acknowledgements are provided in Supplementary Text 3.
Funding
LifeCycle is a 5-year research project that is funded through the European Union’s Horizon 2020 Research and Innovation Programme (grant agreement no. 733206). Cohort‐specific funding is provided in Supplementary Text 2.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
RP, RCH designed the research and statistical approach together with DA. RP performed the analysis with statistical support form DA. RP, RCH and DA was responsible for interpretation of the data, drafted the manuscript and approved the submitted version. All authors contributed contribution to the interpretation of results and revising the manuscript. All authors reviewed and approved the final draft.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participants
All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee. All participants gave written informed consent and ethical approval was granted by local or national ethics committees and provided in Supplementary Text 1.
Consent for publication
Not applicable.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary Information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Pretorius, R.A., Avraam, D., Guxens, M. et al. Is maternal diabetes during pregnancy associated with neurodevelopmental, cognitive and behavioural outcomes in children? Insights from individual participant data meta-analysis in ten birth cohorts. BMC Pediatr 25, 76 (2025). https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12887-024-05365-y
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
DOI: https://doiorg.publicaciones.saludcastillayleon.es/10.1186/s12887-024-05365-y